International Journal of Epidemiology
IJE Advance Access published on January 17, 2007.
doi:10.1093/ije/dyl303
Stanton A Glantz
The placebo effect, when patients report feeling better after receiving
a
treatment even though the treatment has no real benefit, is a
well-known
pitfall in clinical trials. To avoid reporting misleading or incorrect
results in trials, when possible investigators blind the subjects to
the
treatment and seek objective measures of outcome that are unaffected by
the experimental subjects’ beliefs. The need for objective
outcome
measures is particularly important when it is not possible to blind the
experimental subjects. The paper by Adda et al.1 of perceived effects
of
the Scottish smokefree law in this issue of IJE is a textbook example
of
the placebo effect and how collecting subjective data can lead to
unreliable conclusions.
Adda et al. used a quasi-experimental design in which they compared
reported changes in pub business in Scotland a month or so after the
law
went into effect with changes reported across the border in Northern
England. Based on data collected in a telephone survey, they report a
10%
drop in sales and a 14% decrease in customers in Scotland compared with
England...
Analyses need reliable unbiased data to support reliable conclusions
and
avoid the placebo effect. It will be interesting to see if Adda et
al.’s1
conclusions still hold when the objective tax data become available or,
if
as one Philip Morris Tobacco Company lobbyist observed in 1994 when
talking
to her fellow lobbyists, ‘economic arguments often used by the
[tobacco]
industry to scare off smoking ban activity were no longer working, if
indeed they ever did. These arguments simply had no credibility with
the
public, which isn’t surprising when you consider our dire
predictions in
the past rarely came true.’12
Referenced IJE study: [NIMI 5-1-07]
Short-run economic effects of the Scottish smoking ban
http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/...
Source:
IJE